In the
current context of the Research Excellence Framework (REF), there are some
similarities between how academics react to the pressures of the REF and how
crowds respond to novel, pressurised situations.
I find it
interesting that, unlike other ‘human sciences’, psychology is somewhat
fragmented. As an example, biological psychology and social psychology are so
different in their subject matter, assumptions and methods that they hardly
appear to be part of the same discipline. And social psychology itself is often
characterized by differences and deep disagreements over its proper subject
matter (cognition? talk?) and appropriate theoretical framework (social
identity? social cognition?).
These
within-discipline differences become more or less salient according to context.
For example, in the departments I have studied and worked in, I have noticed
that the arguments about what psychology is and should be are often most
evident – most argued over – when the different groups are together in the same
place to hear a guest speaker identifiable with one branch or another. In other
settings – for example when the same people in the same department come
together in a departmental meeting to discuss their teaching and administration
– these differences of perspective on the nature of psychology are often less
evident, and we think and act as a single group.
In the
current context of the REF, psychology departments are being pulled in both
directions – towards fragmentation and towards unity – and the process
resembles in some ways the changes in group identity and alliance – the intergroup
dynamics – I have observed elsewhere.
The REF
context and intergroup dynamics
One of the
things to understand about the REF is that, in common with other drives towards
measurement in the Higher Education sector, it encourages a greater emphasis
than in the past on ‘outputs’. When I got my first research funding, some years
ago, the purpose of the work I was to undertake, I imagined, was to produce
findings that would increase knowledge and develop theory. That is a
traditional view of the nature of academic research. But it is not the way that
we are encouraged to think about our research activity now.
Irrespective
of actual scientific contribution, research grants now need to produce outputs.
What are outputs? They are measurable and hence publicly verifiable products of
the research. The problem is how we define a valuable output. Not all products
of research fit the bill, if we use metrics such as journal impact factors. Journal
articles count as outputs but not all journal articles are equally valued. Even
among all those journals that are peer reviewed in the same subject, such as psychology,
some have better impact factors than others. An impact factor is a calculation
of the journal’s citation rate, but there are different reasons why one journal
has a higher average citation rate than another journal. So, this leaves psychology
in a situation whereby some journals are ranked better than others, if we are
to use journal impact factors.
Journal
impact factors are divisive
It is
well known that social psychology journals have lower impact factors than
psychiatry and neuroscience journals. It is also well known that this
difference is partly a function of different publishing schedules for different
(sub-)disciplines, and the size of the readership or access to the journal, rather
than a reflection of the scientific worth of different kinds of research. The
current REF guidelines tell us that journal impact factors will not be used to evaluate outputs, but
there is a lot of doubt about this pledge. The idea that value depends on a journal’s
impact factor is persistent.
One thing
the emphasis on impact factors has meant in my own university (and I know we
are not unusual, as I have heard the same thing from others) is that social
psychologists are encouraged to publish in general psychology journals or those
from other branches of psychology. This may actually be a good thing. However,
any implication that social psychology journals are inferior, and that
publications in these journals are less worthy, does not seem such a good idea
– either for the morale of researchers or for the development of knowledge.
The
second consequence of this unevenness in the measured value of journals is not
simply in terms of presentation but in terms of content. One psychology department
I know of was advised that they shouldn’t bother with a social psychology group
at all, but instead should invest in more fMRI-based researchers. This puts
some branches of psychology at risk. The other problem with this sort of
approach is, taken to its logical conclusion, leaves psychology not looking too
good relative to other sciences. Given the relevance, for receiving government
funding, of being a STEM discipline, arbitrary rankings can therefore have
serious consequences for the future of psychology as a field.
Pressure
from the REF can be a unifying factor
However,
in this topsy-turvy world, where measurement is to some extent driving the
phenomenon it is meant to measure, there is also a sense in which psychologists
from the different areas of the discipline are all in the same boat. At a departmental
level, there is a shared cynicism about the REF, about all the extra work
involved, and about the pressure on us all to maximize our outputs to meet the
2014 deadline. It is something we complain about together and commiserate collectively
in the tea-room. In this context, therefore, the social world is clearly
organized not in terms of different groups of psychologists with different
ideas about the nature of psychology, but in terms of psychology departments
and the government, with senior management mediating between the two.
This is
not to say that there are not salient REF-related differences within departments,
for someone in the department has to make sure that everyone else is
‘REF-ready’, and in effect to represent the REF to the rest of us and the rest
of us to the REF. On occasions, when I have criticised the REF (and the RAE
before it), I have been reminded by colleagues that to simply ignore it would
be suicidal for a department because of the funding that is bound up with it,
and that we must therefore ‘play the game’. To maintain within-group morale, in
the pressurised climate of the REF, psychologists should reassure their colleagues that they think their work is valued - over
and above their REF rating. One of the findings that has come out of my
research on people in stressful situations (mass emergencies) is that the group
is a key source of survival and recovery through the social support it offers
its members.
(A version of this article was originally published in the BPS Wessex Psychologist Bulletin, No. 8, Spring 2013.)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.